Authors: Preece, Rogers & Sharp
Introduction
Starters
Chapters
Case Studies
Interactivities
Buy the Book [pop-up]
About the Book [pop-up]
2 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Chapter Index
Understanding Users

Chapter Introduction | Web Resources | Assignment Comments | Teaching Materials


When doing this exercise you will probably be surprised at just how little you know about how ATMs work (unless you have worked in a bank). A main reason being that, as far as you are concerned, an ATM is there to serve you efficiently and effectively. You just need to know how to operate it. It is only when unexpected things happen (e.g. it does not give you any money, it says you are overdrawn when you are not, it gives you more money than you asked for) that you may start to wonder how an ATM system works.

Some of the questions we ask in the assignment are difficult to answer. For example, what information is on your card? All you can see is a magnetic strip on the back of the card. You have never been told what is written on it. You have to infer from your knowledge of banking what might be on it. This is likely to include your password, your account number and your limit. But what else?

Most of us are unlikely to have thought much about many of the questions asked in the assignment. So we make inferences on the spot from our limited knowledge about such systems. This is what we mean by ad hoc reasoning. We also will commonly use analogies, e.g. 'well it is like a debit card', in an attempt to explain. Some of these maybe appropriate and others not.

What did you find when you asked other people? You may have discovered that their explanations were quite different. It is quite common for there to be variability between people's explanations of the same system. Also did you find that people used incorrect analogies, superstition and bizarre models to explain their understanding? People can be quite creative when forced to provide explanations when they have no idea!

Another issue that is important to think about is whether you really are eliciting a person's mental model, when asking such questions. How do you know what they say reflects the knowledge they use when interacting with a device? This question has taxed researchers for many years and there is a whole literature on how to elicit mental models and how to determine whether they are really the knowledge representations people use in their activities.

When ATMs first came out, different conceptual models were used by the different banks in the design of their ATMs. A key aspect was whether to (i) let the person take their money first and then give their card back or (ii) give their card back before letting them take their money. Banks that designed their ATMs on the first model found that a surprisingly high number of people forgot to take their card once they had achieved their primary goal of obtaining their cash. Conversely, banks that designed their machines using the second model didn't experience such problems. People rarely left their cash behind once they had removed their card! From an engineering perspective, the order in which they are performed may be regarded as arbitrary (so long as they are both executed). From an interaction design perspective, it is obvious as to why the order in which they are carried out by the user is crucial.